TY - JOUR
T1 - The positive side of a negative reference
T2 - The delay between linguistic processing and common ground
AU - Kronmüller, Edmundo
AU - Noveck, Ira
AU - Rivera, Natalia
AU - Jaume-Guazzini, Francisco
AU - Barr, Dale
N1 - Publisher Copyright:
© 2017 The Authors.
PY - 2017/2/8
Y1 - 2017/2/8
N2 - Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that listeners register the object as well as the identity of the speaker who coined the label. The linguistic view claims that, once established, precedents are treated by listeners like any other linguistic unit, i.e. without needing to keep track of the speaker. To test predictions from each account, we used visual-world eyetracking, which allows observations in real time, during a standard referential communication task. Participants had to select objects based on instructions from two speakers. In the critical condition, listeners sought an object with a negative reference such as not the jellyfish. We aimed to determine the extent to which listeners rely on the linguistic input, common ground or both. We found that initial interpretations were based on linguistic processing only and that common ground considerations do emerge but only after 1000 ms. Our findings support the idea that—at least temporally—linguistic processing can be isolated from common ground.
AB - Interlocutors converge on names to refer to entities. For example, a speaker might refer to a novel looking object as the jellyfish and, once identified, the listener will too. The hypothesized mechanism behind such referential precedents is a subject of debate. The common ground view claims that listeners register the object as well as the identity of the speaker who coined the label. The linguistic view claims that, once established, precedents are treated by listeners like any other linguistic unit, i.e. without needing to keep track of the speaker. To test predictions from each account, we used visual-world eyetracking, which allows observations in real time, during a standard referential communication task. Participants had to select objects based on instructions from two speakers. In the critical condition, listeners sought an object with a negative reference such as not the jellyfish. We aimed to determine the extent to which listeners rely on the linguistic input, common ground or both. We found that initial interpretations were based on linguistic processing only and that common ground considerations do emerge but only after 1000 ms. Our findings support the idea that—at least temporally—linguistic processing can be isolated from common ground.
KW - Common ground
KW - Dialogue
KW - Language
KW - Negation
KW - Reference
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=85012094084&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.1098/rsos.160827
DO - 10.1098/rsos.160827
M3 - Article
AN - SCOPUS:85012094084
SN - 2054-5703
VL - 4
JO - Royal Society Open Science
JF - Royal Society Open Science
IS - 2
M1 - 160827
ER -